Skip to content
My WebMD Sign In, Sign Up

Brain & Nervous System Health Center

Font Size

Why We Love or Hate Certain Smells

It's Nurture, Not Nature, That Lets Us Smell the Roses

WebMD Health News

Jan. 6, 2005 -- At last! The reason that some of us love the smell of mothballs and others can't stand it.

Researchers from Brown University in Providence, R.I., report that our responses to certain odors are based on past experiences with the scent.

"Most people assume we all like the smell of roses and hate the smell of skunk," lead researcher Rachel Herz, PhD, a visiting assistant professor of psychology at Brown, tells WebMD. But "with the exception of irritating odors, smell is something we come to know from personal and cultural experiences."

In one experiment of 30 women, participants played a computer game in the presence of a custom-made, slightly unpleasant scent that mixed dirt, rain, and hot buttered popcorn. If participants had a good time playing the game, they were more likely to report liking the odor they smelled. If they didn't like the game, they didn't like the scent.

"We come to smell with a blank slate, and why we like or dislike it has to do with experiences," Herz says.

The study appears in the December 2004 issue of the International Journal of Comparative Psychology.

In the second experiment, 36 men and women tested two new scents, one slightly floral, the other clean and watery. In a pretest, they rated these scents as well as rose, vanilla, lemon, and peppermint. Participants rated the new scents as both unfamiliar and pleasant.

But not for long.

One group entered a scented room and played a frustrating computer card game replete with annoying sound effects and losing hands. By contrast, another group sat in a scented room and read magazines. In both groups, half were exposed to the floral scent, the other half to the watery one. The third group played the computer game in an odorless room. All participants either played the computer game or read magazines in three sessions spread out over the course of a week. After each session, they rated the six scents.

Participants were more likely to score the pleasing new odors as distasteful after playing the frustrating game.

The bottom line from both experiments is that when an odor is paired with an emotional event, perception of that odor is altered to fit that association.

"This study contributes quite a lot to the study of smell because very little has been done in terms of any direct experiments that look at how or why it is we like/dislike smells," she says.

Although little empirical data exist, Herz says, cultural studies back up the new results. Consider that Americans tend to like the smell of wintergreen, a common ingredient in candy and gum, but in Britain, where wintergreen is often used to make medicine, the odor is less pleasing.

There are, however, a few exceptions to this theory. Irritating odors, such as ammonia, may be immediately disliked when smelled, and individual genetic differences may also play a role in emotional responses to odors.

Today on WebMD

Depressed
Slideshow
3d scan of fractured skull
Slideshow
 
human brain waves
Article
brain maze
fitQuiz
 
senior man
Article
brain research briefing
Article
 
Syringe
Article
Vaccine and needle
VIDEO
 
mans hands on laptop keyboard
Article
brain illustration stroke
Slideshow
 
most common stroke symptoms
Article
Parkinsons Disease Medications
Article